From: elmeras2000
Message: 35319
Date: 2004-12-05
> Moreover, in my opinion it's a question of _initial_ accentYou appear to have the language users on your side - they too found
> vs. _final_ accent (not merely final vs. non-final, which is
> too abstract for my taste). While *dhug&2té:r an its
> paradigm played a role in establishing the pattern for the
> oxytone forms, the example of *h2ákmo:n and *nébhos and such
> was just as important for establishing the pattern for the
> barytone forms of the mobile paradigm.
> >> 2) Hirt's law. A non-vocalic laryngeal in the firstabout
> >> syllable attracts the stress. Raises the number of
> >> barytones.
> >>
> >> [3) Winter's law. Causes acute tone, but does not (usually)
> >> result in retraction of the stress, so must come after
> >> Hirt's]
> >
> >Only if the prosodic trigger was the same in the two cases. Since
> >that does not have to be so, nothing seems to be really known
> >that point. It will be an argument only to those who assumeThen what *did* you imply? What caused you to use the fact that
> >that "glottalization" had coalesced with laryngeals. How they can
> >believe they know that is beyond me.
>
> I wasn't implying anything of the kind. I do observe that
> both Winter's law and laryngeals result in acute prosody.
> But that includes vocalized laryngeals that do *not* trigger
> Hirt's law, so there is no basis even _without_ Winter's
> law.
> >> The above were Balto-Slavic. Now for the Slavic laws:syllable.
> >>
> >> 4) Dybo's law. Non-acute barytones become mesotonic
> >> (AP(b)).
> >
> >Dybo's law moved the accent from a non-acute to the next
> >Mobile paradigms were immune to it (speakers of BSl. LOVEmobility),
> >but the law worked in non-mobile paradigms and in isolated words.vowels,
> >
> >> 5) Meillet's law. Mobile paradigms lose their original
> >> stress (acute, circumflex or short) in the non-oxytone forms
> >> and become enclinomenic ["unstressed"] vs. oxytone.
> >
> >Mobility is extended to work even on the components of long
> >so that initial accent yields a falling tone (first-mora accent)if
> >the paradigm is mobile. The extension to the accentual unit ofthe
> >sentence is not part of Meillet's findings, but that occurredtoo.
> >Thomas has shown me that it may well be common to the entirety ofbut
> >Balto-Slavic and so be much older than this.
> >
> >> 6) Stang's law. Stress is retracted from weak yers and
> >> (circumflex) long medial vowels, resulting in neo-acute
> >> accent on the preceding syllable.
> >>
> >> 7) Neo-circumflex and other accentual changes are
> >> post-Common Slavic.
> >
> >I don't think they can be. Stang just says the "circumflex" in
> >question is not phonetically diifferent from other circumflexes,
> >that is quite beyond the point he should be making. The fact isthat
> >a circumflex appears where an acute was expected, unless some Pan-should
> >Slavic change of acute to neo-circumflex is accepted. So that
> >be accepted.I don't think we can, for the languages agree too closely. The fact
>
> I haven't looked at the neocircumflex much yet, so correct
> me if I'm wrong. The impression I got from browsing trough
> that part of "Osnovy" is that in S.Cr., Slov. and West
> Slavic (as far as can be observed) we have a circumflex
> instead of an acute in certain positions, most notably in
> the G.pl. (*-U:), to a lesser degree also before lengthened
> thematic -e:-, and to an even lesser degree before other
> "old" long endings, as well as before new contractions
> (-ojoN > -o:/-u:, -aja > -a:) and lengthenings (to do with
> loss of yers). These last circumstances clearly point to a
> post-Common Slavic timeframe, and from the point of view of
> what I am planning to do (which is try to reproduce and
> verify the accentual laws automatically with the Sound
> Change Applier), I think I can safely ignore the
> neo-circumflex, as long as I get the lengths right.