From: Sean Whalen
Message: 34594
Date: 2004-10-11
> On 04-10-08 21:44, Sean Whalen wrote:I agree PIE needs to be constructed by comparison
>
> > Comparative? This is mainly an internal
> > reconstruction.
>
> It was in Saussure's time, when the "sonant
> coefficients" were used
> mainly for explaining minor ablaut patterns, but now
> it's as comparative
> as anything else: you can directly compare things
> like Anatolian aitches
> with Greek prothetic vowels, Indo-Iranian
> aspiration, failure of
> Brugmann's Law, Balto-Slavic intonations etc. It all
> amounts to good
> comparative evidence.
> For example, assuming f at an early stage, whatI've never heard this evidence; s could easily have
> > happened to it later? If f>s, does the treatment
> of s
> > appear different in any two environments, and thus
> can
> > be attributed to two underlying sounds? Since I
> had
> > already decided -a'ms>-a'm_>-a:'m to account for
> > n/m/s/etc.-stems' nom. sng. but thematic acc. pl.
> a'ms
> > didn't change, probably pl. -f. This further was
> > confirmed by -fi>-fu in loc. pl., not explainable
> by
> > an unrounded C (as loc. sng. -(a)wi>-(a)wu).
>
> You ignore the rather solid evidence (defended here
> eloquently by Jens
> Rasmussen) that the nom.sg. *-s was once a voiced
> segment). The crucial
> part of the demonstration is the behavious of the
> thematic vowel, with
> its *-e/o- allomorphy clearly governed by the
> phonation of the following
> segment. If thing are so, the difference was between
> *z and *s at least
> at the time when the *-e/o- rule was operative.
> > Similarly, if palatal(ized) nj existed, whatAthematic imperfect affix -n(e)- and thematic
> did it
> > change into? Probably either n or y. Two of the
> most
> > common V affixes are n and y, could they be the
> same,
> > and in what environment would n become either?
> Also,
> > suyus/sunus. I determined laws that work to do
> > describe this.
>
> Well, there are arguments for the original identity
> of some instances of
> alternating *n and *r, or some instances of *s and
> *t, or *n and *t, or
> even *h1 and *t -- all of them still tentative at
> this stage. One
> problem for your account is that there is no
> functional relationship
> between *-jo- and *-no- (as opposed e.g. to *-n�-
> and *-t�-, which both
> form quasi-participles, or to *-r/-n- in the
> heteroclita). I'd also like
> to see your detailed explanation of the 'son' words
> before I believe
> that they are phonologically conditioned variants of
> the same underlying
> protoform (rather than related words with different
> derivational histories).
> >>The fact that *h3Well, I don't know much about this, so all I can
> >>seems to have had a voicing effect on any
> preceding
> >>stop suggests that
> >>it was voiced itself... etc.
>
> > Since all the fricatives are voiceless, it's
> not
> > unlikely one or all had voiced allophones. If not
> > before, they were almost certainly voiced when
> they
> > became syllabic.
>
> The whole point about *h3 is that it seems to have
> been _distinctively_
> voiced. Otherwise the voicing would not have any
> assimilatory effect, as
> visible in *h2ap-h3on- > *abon- or *pi-ph3-e-ti >
> *pibeti.
> Judging fromWell, no stops have any effect on all vowels. It's
> their phonetic effects, the three laryngeals were
> something like *h, *x,
> and *G (the latter two in the
> velar-uvular-pharyngeal range) rather than
> anything parallel to *k^, *k, *kW. Even the O-colour
> of *h3 was not
> necessarily a consequence of lip-rounding: note that
> *kW, *gW and *gWH
> don't colour any *e's in PIE!
> > I think o/e/0 shifts make it almost certain thereBut the features I have are found in other
> > was one vowel in the past that changed sound for
> some
> > reason, tone seems to do it.
>
> Here I suppose most people (including myself) will
> agree with you in
> principle. The question is if we have any right to
> reconstruct
> distinctive tone values for PIE just in order to
> account for the basic
> ablaut of *e : *o. If we do, we don't really explain
> anything -- it's a
> case of "obscurum per obscurius". If you write that
> nouns were mostly
> marked with one of the tones and verbs with the
> other, you're only
> restating what everyone knows -- that nouns often
> show the o-grade and
> verbs often show the e-grade. I can't see any
> simplification in your
> model. It trades off one kind of complexity against
> another.
> > See some of the derivations I sent for theThe presence of tones explains many things, not
> > different cases of "sister" and "dog". If
> swa`fa'r-
> > is the underlying stem, gen. affix -a's deletes
> tone
> > to get swa`far- and the toneless a will be
> affected by
> > later a-deletion rules. When simplification
> causes
> > only one tone per word: swe'sros. The tone of the
> > final syllable is the only tone for kuxva'n-, so:
> > kuxvno's. By analogy with words with only final
> tone
> > (common), morphemes like -su'/-su (and all
> tone-marked
> > plural affixes) are simplified to their most
> common
> > form, thus swesrsu' instead of swe'srsu.
>
> The same forms are adequately accounted for in some
> derivational models
> (discussed here more than once in the past) that
> only have recourse to
> independently required lexical accent (surfacing as
> stress) and don't
> employ primary tonal contrasts. They are superior to
> your tonal solution
> by being more parsimonious.