From: Richard Wordingham
Message: 26342
Date: 2003-10-11
> Dear all,few
>
> I have learned from one book on historical linguistics that the
> English perfect aspect(has/have V-en) is derived from a stative,
> possessive construction, i.e. from a construction like 'I have a
> ribs broken.' According to the author, there is no indication thatI
> did the breaking. And the sentence means 'I have a few brokenribs.'
>English
> Later, this construction was reanalyzed, or reinterpreted, in a
> perfective sense. Hence it is the origin of the present-day
> perfective aspect.main
> Now, I have encountered a curious sentence as follows:
>
> Ac hie haefdon tha¡Khiora mete genotudne
> But they had then¡Ktheir food used-up
> 'But they had then used up their food.'
>
> The sentence can be only interpreted as a perfect construction
> because something, being used up, cannot be possessed. That means
> what had been the original main verb(haefdon) was now an auxiliary
> and what had been an adjectival function(genotudne) was now the
> verb.verb
>
> However, something about this sentence deserves our attention: the
> suffix is attached to the verb 'genotudn-.' That is, the main
> now agrees with the object 'mete' only!languages
> Isn't this phenomenon a counterexample against the implicational
> universal that states that if a particular phenomenon applies to
> direct objects, it should also apply to subjects?
> Ths implicational universal also predicts that there are no
> in which the verb agrees with just the direct object. But in theretained
> above case, the universal doesn't seem correct.
>
> I understand the behaviour demonstrated by the English perfect
> construction was at a transitional stage. Therefore it still
> a residue of the possessive construction.evidence
> But is it possible for a language at a transitional stage to go
> against the implicational universal? Or the sentence was not
> against the universal at all?But this rule is not a brief transitional rule at the Académie