From: Piotr Gasiorowski
Message: 4969
Date: 2000-12-07
>Why should the 3-waydistinction have had to be preserved *somewhere* for it to have been real?
counterargument.>>(Re: the relative rarity of *K) I’ve never seen a convincing
>Actually, your (3) is one. "There is a bias inthe Brugmannian reconstruction: anything that has a *$ reflex in any Satem language is automatically assigned to the *K^ set."
>Undoubtedly, in some environments (in some languages), the oppositionbetwen *K and *K^ is neutralized. So is, in some environments (in some langages), but to a lesser degree, the opposition between *K and *Kw, but that doesn't mean we should consider them positional allophones of a single phoneme (although it's of course easy, perhaps
>Ido not deny that in the satem area the boundary between *K and *K^ becomes fuzzy. But it does not follow that this reflects a split of *K into *K^ (normal development) and *K (in "blocking environments" etc.). It can also mean a near-merger between *K^ and *K (with *K^ swallowing up large portions of earlier *K, esp. in I-I).
>The number of cases where, even in I-I, onefinds "unmotivated" *K (no *r following, etc.) is high enough for me to prefer the latter option. Hell, I've even gone so far as claiming that the loss of the distinction between *K, *K^ and *Kw is nothing but the final stage of a process whereby a three-way split in quality of *all* consonants was
>[...] Just in case I wasn't clear enough, I'll stateagain that these "blocking environments" are far too ill-defined ("loose") for my taste.
Kortlandt, Frederik. 1978. "IE palatovelars before resonants in Balto-Slavic". In: Jacek Fisiak (ed.). Recent Developments in Historical Phonology. The Hague: Mouton.Kortlandt compares the Balto-Slavic distribution of the reflexes of "*K^" with those in Indo-Iranian, Albanian and Armenian with all the scrupulousness of a Leiden scholar, and his findings corroborate Meillet's approach. I find his analysis convincing. The only point where I disagree with Kortlandt is his insistence that the two PIE articulations were *k^ and *kW. This would require independent and hard-to-motivate shifts in the Kentum branches.As for the number of *K words, I'd say that it's amazingly low for what is apparently the unmarked subset of a natural class. If loanwords and onomatopoeic words are excluded, the are very few good cases left. The rarity of "unmotivated" *K is striking in any lexical field. I mentioned PIE numerals in my posting, but the same is true of body parts, elements of nature, names of animals etc. Look at this:*K^ -- *k^uo:n 'dog', *k^asos 'hare', *(h1)ek^wos 'horse', *pork^os 'pig(let)', *(h1)elk^is 'elk', *pek^u 'livestock', *g^Hwe:r 'wild animal', *g^Hans 'goose', *h2rtk^os 'bear', *h1eg^His 'hedgehog', *dHg^Huhs 'fish'... (nothing recherché about them)*KW -- *wlkWos 'wolf', *gWo:us 'cow', h2agWnos 'lamb', ...*K -- *gerh2- 'crane' (how much longer can you make this inventory)?If we exclude a handful of "North-Western" words that are found in Balto-Slavic but not elsewhere in Satem and have Germanic, Italic and/or Celtic cognates (*dHrougHos 'companion', *gHostis 'guest', etc.), there remain several problematic cases like *legH- 'lie' or *dlhgHos 'long', but not too many of them. If the shift *K > *$ began somewhere within the Satem area and spread through lexical diffusion, it would be hardly surprising if we found, say, 80% of consistently shifted items, 15% of forms that vary between *K and *$, and a 5% residue of *K words.Brugmannian *K^ is also significantly more frequent than *KW, which squares well with the interpretation of *K^ (= my *K) as the unmarked member of the opposition *K : *KW. This is what we find in Kentum. During the Satem dorsal shift the markedness values were reversed and a new *K (reflecting the merger of PIE *KW with unshifted old *K) became the unmarked ("plain") dorsal.
>And I'll add that a three-way distinction between palatalized, plain and labialized/velarized consonants is in no way typologically implausible, although it would seem to be diachronically instable (and thus not terribly common). A good example is Old Irish, where besides the well-known "broad" and "slender" (palatalized) consonant qualities, there was also "u-colouring" (labio-velarization), caused by (formerly) following *u or *u:. Even in the oldest Old Irish glosses, "u-quality" was losing ground to "neutral quality" (maybe it was more of a merger, given the velarized quality of the Gaelic "broad" consonants). The same may have happened in Slavic, where the loss of the yers <I> and <U> may have initially resulted in (the phonologization of) three consonant qualities, later resolved into two (palatalized vs. plain [though often labio-velarized, especially /l/]) or even back to one in modern Slavic languages.
Such a contrast is indeed possible, though typological considerations would make one expect some place-dependent asymmetry in the system. All kinds of secondary articulation go very well with dorsals and almost equally well with coronals; "labial" is typologically the least frequently modified articulation, and labials with extra lip rounding as a distinctive feature seem to occur only in very special circumstances (e.g. in systems with no contrastive vowel roundness, as in Aranda).As for your Slavic scenario, if you can show some evidence (preferably a set of minimal pairs) for a ternary contrast like *t : *t' : *tW, we can discuss it.Piotr