PIE dorsals

From: Piotr Gasiorowski
Message: 4963
Date: 2000-12-06

The three series of dorsals, *K^, *K and *KW (*K = *k/*g/*gH), were proposed by several scholars, including Osthoff, in the 1870s and accepted by Brugmann, whose reputation as the man who'd codified ‘standard PIE’ secured them a position of permanence in PIE reconstructions:

PIE                Satem   Kentum
palatovelar *K^  >  *$      *K
plain velar *K   >  *K      *K
labiovelar  *KW  >  *K      *KW

... where *$ is a cover symbol for the various Satem reflexes of *K^.

According to one dissident view, defended by Antoine Meillet, PIE had only *K and *KW, and the apparent contrasts on which the Brugmannian reconstruction of *K^ is based resulted from the split of *K into palatal and non-palatal variants in different contexts in the Satem languages. The main weakness of this approach is that those ‘different contexts’ are difficult to define. It seems as though Meillet’s *K series had both palatal and non-palatal Satem reflexes in certain environments, in particular before *r.

Another unorthodox theory (Møller and Kuryłowicz), claims that the original series were *K^ and *K, whereas *KW developed from *K -- mainly before front vowels, through some kind of dissimilatory process -- as a Kentum innovation. There are acute problems with this scheme, too, e.g. the occurrence of labiovelars before consonants and the lack of typological precedent for the proposed developments. It is extremely unlikely that such an odd change should have occurred independently in the various Kentum branches. However, in order to posit it as a common innovation one would be forced to make the indefensible assumption that the Kentum languages are more closely related to one another than any of them is to any Satem language, or that they used to form a cohesive convergence area.

I don’t think this theory can be maintained. The real choice is between Brugmann and Meillet. Let me begin by listing the most powerful arguments against the classic Brugmannian aproach.

(1) No IE language preserves contrastive reflexes of all the three series. This suggests that the ‘K-overlap’ (Satem *K and Kentum *K in the same root) may represent a neutralisation zone between two original series.

(2) In a system with three dorsal articulations the ‘neutral’ (unmarked) one (*K) should be at least as common as any of the complex (marked) ones. This is a general property of natural classes of phonemes, and any violation of it should be commented on and, if possible, accounted for. But in PIE the *K series (across phonation types) is definitely the rarest, and many of the examples of *k, *g and *gH cited in the literature are too doubtful for profitable discussion. In the basic vocabulary *K^ and *KW are overwhelmingly more common. For example, *k^ occurs in the words for 6, 8 and 10 (not to mention the decads and 100), *kW in 4 and 5, whereas no number words contain *k.

(3) In the Satem languages there is a good deal of variation between *K and *$. Even in Indo-Iranian *K for expected *$ is sometimes found, and in Balto-Slavic non-satemised *K is very common. The opposite (*$ for expected *K) is virtually impossible for purely technical reasons. There is a bias in the Brugmannian reconstruction: anything that has a *$ reflex in any Satem language is automatically assigned to the *K^ set. One should note, however, that the contrast between *K^ and *KW in Satem is disturbed unidirectionally: some instances of PIE *K^ are reflected as *K, but PIE *KW never emerges as *$. To sum up, the boundary between *K and *K^ is fuzzy, while the *KW lexical set preserves its integrity in th Satem languages.
Possible counterarguments:

(ad 1) It has been claimed that Albanian and Luwian retain the three-way contrast in some positions. The Albanian evidence was first presented by Holger Pedersen early in this century. Now that Albanian historical phonology has made substantial progress, Pedersen’s argument has lost its force -- notwithstanding which, Albanian is regularly mentioned by Ivanov as a language reflecting ‘three rows of velars’, also in his UCLA lectures on Advanced IE Phonology this very quarter:

http://www.humnet.ucla.edu/pies/IES205FQ00.html

The attention of linguists has shifted to Luwian, which has the advantage of being a dead and imperfectly known language, so any claim about it, no matter how odd, may be hard to refute. There are just a few instances of Luwian <z> [ts] that might correspond to PIE *k^. In most cases <z> is followed by <i>, which suggests a trivial palatalisation like Proto-Anatolian *ki > tsi (as in the putative Luwian and Lycian reflexes of *k^(e)i- ‘lie, rest’). One remarkable exception is Luwian za- ‘this’ vs. Hittite and Palaic ka:-, but even this may be secondary (analogical generalisation of the regular [ts] that arose in zi-). Anyway, Hieroglyphic Luwian luha- ‘light’ and dakam- ‘ground’ show velar reflexes (also in Cuneiform Luwian tijamni- we have lenition but no evidence of affrication).

One ought to remember that velars are particularly susceptible to palatalisation and may go through several cycles of ‘softening’ in the history of a single linguistic lineage (Slavic and Romance provide striking examples). Brugmannian *k^ often ends up as French [s] (as in cent) before front vowels, while *k yields <ch> [S] when palatalised before Proto-Romance *a (as in chevre or chien), and *kW is not palatalised (qui, quatre). This might easily result in a mirage of a three-way contrast if French were a little-known and poorly documented language.

(ad 2) I’ve never seen a convincing counterargument.

(ad 3) If we lump *K^ and *K together, how shall we account for the stable *K that occurs in a number of roots (e.g. *kr(e)uh2- ‘blood; raw meat’: Latin cruor, Old English hre:aw, Greek kreas, Sanskrit kravis.-, Old Polish kry, etc.)?

There are some loose regularities here. *K tends to occur before non-syllabic *r (in Baltic also before *l) both word-initially and medially. Indo-Iranian and Armenian introduce $ where there is analogical support for it, but retain *K otherwise (cf. Middle English kerven ‘carve’ < OE c^eorfan [tSe@...] influenced by pret.pl. curfon [kurvon], pp. corfen [korven]). Balt-Slavic languages have uniform *Kr. I’d like to suggest that non-palatal *K is also the normal Satem reflex before *h2 [x] as in *dHugh2ter-, and probably after *s.
 
Only *K is found in suffixes such as *-(i)k- and *-(i)sk- presumably because only the ‘basic’ velar was allowed in word-forming suffixes already in PIE and suffixal *-k- did not contrast with *-kW-. In many words (especially in Balto-Slavic) apparent ‘dispalatalisation’ must be due to lexical borrowing from Centum languages. My favourite example of a similar phenomenon is the ‘restoration’ of formerly palatalised velars in English words like egg, get, give, score, etc., thanks to Old Norse influence. This would easily account for a number of cases like Slavic *goNsI- (*gansi-) ‘goose’ vs. Baltic *z^ansi-.

What can’t be defended is *ok^to: < **okito: as proposed by Szemerényi. There is no independent evidence of syncope in such words; besides, Satem *$ is too frequent in environments where palatalisation caused by front vowels can be ruled out. It must be concluded that the Satem palatalisation was not restricted to a particular context (although it was blocked in some environments). Such a shift is hardly the usual thing that happens to velars, but fortunately the Satem grouping (as opposed to Centum) has always been geographically cohesive (I’d also argue that it’s possibly monophyletic) and it’s quite possible that ‘Satemisation’ was a single event rather than a number of independent developments in several branches.

My preferred scenario is therefore as follows:

THE SATEM SHIFT

PIE *K  > Satem *$ (except in blocking environments, where *K is retained)

PIE *KW > Satem *K

... while Centum languages simply preserve the PIE state of things.

It’s getting so late that if I started taking issue with G & I’s root-structure argument I wouldn’t be able to finish tonight. I’ll return to it later, but -- in case you wonder -- my opinion is that the examples cited in support of it are for the most part artificial and that the whole argument is entirely specious.

Piotr